Wednesday, December 14, 2011

Final Post: Superiority of Folk Knowledge


Throughout this semester, we have investigated four different types of knowledge: folk knowledge, oral knowledge, written knowledge, and print knowledge. Folk knowledge we defined as knowledge that you come by informally, usually from some kind of expert. Examples of folk knowledge can include how to breed bunnies, how to crack a bull whip, and how to play an instrument or a sport. Due to the nature of this unique type of knowledge, folk knowledge accommodates collaboration and building a community more than other types of knowledge.


Because folk knowledge requires personal, face-to-face interaction, it facilitates community building more than other types of knowledge. Humans are social creatures, and shared experiences create the sense of community. In Catherine’s post about the Plains Indians, she described how those people didn’t waste a single scrap of the beasts they hunted. Passing that kind of knowledge from one generation to the next required the kind of collaboration and interaction typical of folk knowledge, and helped to create their tight-knit community.  I also experienced the effects of folk knowledge in my blogging group when we got together to teach each other various types of folk knowledge. We each took a turn: Blaine Harker taught each of us how to swing a Hungarian bullwhip, Catherine Hawkley taught us how to braid hair, and Holland Hettinger taught us how to make delicious chocolate chip cookies. Learning from and interacting with each other helped to build our sense of community as we shared those common experiences.

Do other types of knowledge facilitate collaboration and community building to the extent that folk knowledge does? I submit that they do not. Although oral knowledge accommodates collaboration, shared oral-learning experiences do not build as tight-knit a community as does shared folk knowledge. For example, even though I attend the same lectures bi-weekly as the other forty members of this class, I feel less of a sense of community with these fellow class mates than I do with my particular blogging group because I have very little interaction with the other class members. In much the same way, the ancient Polynesian peoples, who enjoyed a strong oral tradition, experienced a greater sense of community from folk-knowledge activities such as hunting, fishing, and fighting together than they did from listening to their stories. It is clear that this is so because the people of the various island nations were constantly at war with each other even though they shared the same stories and myths.

As Dane pointed out in the Salon yesterday, written and printed knowledge allow greater dispersion of knowledge, which can allow for greater collaboration, but because these mediums allow the author to become more detached and removed, they do not facilitate community building to the same degree as folk knowledge. Just as the father tongue distances the speaker from his topic, written and printed knowledge inhibit the kind of community-building, mother tongue interactions available during the diffusion of folk knowledge.

Folk knowledge, though most primitive of the types of knowledge we have studied, does more to facilitate and accommodate collaboration and community-building than do other types of knowledge. It’s also interesting to see that that is the kind of knowledge we experience most in our families and friends, which also attests to its community-focused nature.

No comments:

Post a Comment